Hydrochoerus hydrochaeris - Capybara
Skull Pictures:
Dorsal Ventral
Lateral Dentary
Photos
by Christopher Yahnke, collection of the Field Museum of Natural History
Description:
Hydrochaeris hydrochaeris, the Capybara, is
the largest member of the Family Rodentia. It has short limbs ending
in perissodactyle feet and moderately webbed digits. The forefeet are
characterized by having four digits while the hind feet have three.
Both fore and hindfeet’s nails are hoof-like, round, and thick. (Mones
and Ojasti 1986) Unlike some rodents the capybara does not have a tail.
(Kato 2002) The Capybara has a broad head with short rounded ears, a
large snout, and small widely separated nostrils. This large rodent is
covered in course hair that measures from 30-120 mm. It ranges from
dark brown to reddish and light brown to light yellowish gray. The hair
covers sweat glands that are unique to the capybara and make it the
only rodent to have sweat glands on haired surfaces. Hydrochaeris
hyrochaeris weighs in between 35 to 65.5 kg on average. Males and
females are distinguished (externally) by the nasal glands that are
present in males (their presence indicates the capybara is sexually
mature (Kato 2002)) and by the six pairs of ventral mammae the females
possess. The genitals of both sexes are hidden by anal sacs making it
very hard to tell the sexes apart based on external genitalia. The
incisors of the males are generally larger and broader than those of
females that are the same age. (Mones and Ojasti 1986)
Distribution:
In general the Capybara is found from
Panama down to northern Argentina. It dwells in a variety of habitats,
but is generally found near a water source (standing or running). The
capybara can also be found in lowland rainforests, dry forests, scrub,
grasslands near water, areas with seasonal flooding, and areas with
permanent water sources. (Ziegler, Unger, Feiler, Kraft 2002) Their
home range is approximately 10 to 15 hectares depending on the area,
vegetation, and distance to a water source. (Lord 1993)
Specifically in Paraguay the exact distribution is not clear when
referring to the Gran Chaco area. The upper Chaco (the northwestern
area), also called the Dry Chaco, is considered an area where capybaras
are very unlikely to be found. Areas in Paraguay containing the
Paraguay/ Parana rivers are better suited habitats for the capybara.
These rivers primarily run outside of the Dry Chaco. It is cited in
Ziegler, Unger, Feiler, and Kraft 2002, that the capybara does inhabit
eastern and southern Dry Chaco areas, which contradicts the theory that
the species doesn’t occur in the Dry Chaco. The exact habitat and range
of the capybara in the Dry Chaco is still being researched. (Ziegler,
Unger, Feiler, Kraft 2002) (Mares 1982)
Ontogeny and
Reproduction:
The capybara becomes sexually mature when it reaches a body mass of 30
kg which usually correlates to age 1.5. The females usually have
between one to eight young per gestation. Paraguay’s capybaras
generally have between one to four young; making their litter size
smaller than capybaras in other regions. Mating generally occurs around
the onset of the rainy season which begins about April or May. If the
habitat is good capybaras may breed more than once a year. (Mones and
Ojasti 1986)
The gestation period lasts a proximally 120 days which puts parturition
in September to November. (Kato 2002) Capybaras do not give birth in a
nest, as some rodents do, but give birth anywhere in their habitat where
cover is available. In areas with little to no cover the young are
susceptible to predation from avian predators. Predators of young
include: caracaras, black vultures, and caimans. The young are born
with a complete fur covering and complete permanent dentition. They
will suckle off the mother’s mammae for three to four months, but milk
is not their main source of nutrients. Grasses are the major
contributor to the capybara’s diet in both young and adults. (Mones and
Ojasti 1986)
Ecology and Behavior:
The capybara primarily occupies habitats
that contain water. They can be found along most riverbanks, swamp
land, wetlands, forested areas, and former riverbeds. As long as there
is a source of water for the capybara to use they will occupy that
area. Capybaras use water for drinking, wallowing, and for protection
against terrestrial predators. They also use the water as a way to cool
themselves when the temperatures are hot (the density of their sweat
glands is low resulting in a limited ability to cool themselves by
sweating). The mud near the water source is used as a defense against
infestation by acarids. (Lord 1993)
Capybaras require land for grazing and dry land to rest upon. During
the rainy season capybaras will use the full area of their habitat and
spread out to graze. When the dry season returns they congragate to the
water source in response to the lack of resources and food. The dry
season usually also brings the reduction of the capybara population due
to starvation, increased predation (as a result of the increased
capybara population during the rainy season), and disease. (Mones and
Ojasti 1986)
In general the capybara will take in about 70 grams of grass a day from
grazing. They are selective grazers and tend to select plants rich in
protein. Capybaras eat both terrestrial and aquatic plants during their
grazing periods. Once the plant is eaten the capybara swallows; later
while resting it will regurgitate its food and masticate. Capybaras
have a single chambered stomach unlike many mammals that practice
rumination. (Lord 1993) During the dry season, when food sources are
scarce, capybaras may move into livestock areas and compete with them
for food resources. (Mones and Ojasti 1986)
Capybaras can run, walk, and swim. Even though they are rodents they
are excellent swimmers and divers. Young capybaras tend to stay out of
the water. Their behavior suggests that they are aware of the dangers
in the water and prefer to graze on land. (Lord 1993) Capybaras can
only run for 300 meters and tend to walk from grazing area to grazing
area in a straight line along paths the herd has established. (Mones
and Ojasti 1986)
The herds range from one pair, to a family, to even complex groups with
many adults (male and female) and their young. Size of capybara herds
change throughout the year; medium size (5.6) during the rainy season
and highest in the drier months (15.9). There is usually one dominant
male, a few adult females (ranked according to their weights), their
offspring, and a few subordinate males making up a herd. (Mones and
Ojasti 1986) The subordinate males tend to stay on the edge of the core
social group (in small groups of their own numbering two to four) and
are often the target of the dominate males pursuits and aggression.
These same dominate males are never seen acting with aggression toward
females, young, or infants. Young capybaras do play and act with
aggression toward one another and are often seen mock copulating with
other litter mates. (Lord 1993)
Mating rituals entail a male closely following a female on shore and in
the water. When she stops in the shallower water he mounts her for six
to ten fast thrusts. This act can be repeated many times with the same
or a different partner. (Mones and Ojasti 1986) It ends with the
female capybara retreating from the male. (Lord 1993) Mating occurs
during the rainy season, but in plentiful habitats it may occur more
than once a year. (Mones and Ojasti 1986)
Marking the territories of the capybara is the responsibility of the
dominate male. He uses the sebaceous glands on his snout and rubs
bushes, stems, and plants. Females, young, and subordinate males also
use their sebaceous glands to make rubbings, but don’t do it as
frequently as dominate males. Urine is also used as to mark the
territories of the capybara. Both males and female use sacs located
next to the anus to urinate on brush and other plant material. Marking
aids the herd by alerting other capybara groups (groups tend not to
intermingle) to their territory and helps the herd itself stay within
its own territory. (Lord 1993)
Capybara calls are unique to the rodent. When a predator is close or
approaching they will let out a loud bark. This will continue until the
predator leaves or the capybaras retreat into the water for
protection. Whistle calls are made by young or infants when in need of
their mother and will not terminate unless the mother is seen and
returns. Adult females also make this call when seeking an adult male.
Similarly the female will not stop until the male has begun to
approach. Lastly the “chuckle” is heard among members of the heard
(adult females and young) while traveling together in a group. (Lord
1993)
Remarks:
Adult capybara’s have very few predators;
the main one being humans. Capybaras are hunted (often over hunted) for
their hides, meat, or just for being a rodent and a pest. South
Americans harvest 10,000+ hides annually for items such as gloves,
belts, shoes, handbags, and many other leather pieces. (Mones and Ojasti
1986) Capybara meat is considered below standard in most areas, but a
few select areas in South America (such as Venezuela) see the meat as a
great source of protein. (Mones and Ojasti 1986) The fat from the
capybara is also used for medicinal purposes such as respiratory
illnesses. (Kato 2002) It is for these reasons that many ranchers
welcome the capybara onto their ranches and expect to make a profit from
them living on their land.
(Mones and Ojasti 1986)
Literature Cited:
Mones, Alvaro. and Ojasti, Junani. Hydrocoerus hydrochaeris. June 1986. American Society of Mammalogists. Mammalian Species No. 264, pp. 1-7, 3 figs.
Lord, R. D. (1993): A Descriptive Account of Capybara Behavior. Studies on Neotropical Fauna and Environment 29, (1994), pp.11-22
Ziegler, Thomas. Unger, Jakob. Feiler, Alfred. and Kraft, Richard. Faunistische Abhandlungen. May 2002. On the occurrence of the capybara, Hydrochoerus hydrochaeris (Linnaues, 1766) in the Dry Chaco of Paraguay (Mammalia: Rodentia: Hydrochoeridae), pp. 424-429
Mares, Michael A. and Genoways, Hugh M. 1982. Mammalian Biology in South America. Pp. 393
Neris, Nora. Colman, Flaviano. Ovelar, Enrique. Sukigara, Naozumi. Ishi, Nobuo. 2002. Kato’s Data Book on Larger Mammals of Paraguay, Distribution, Population Trends and Utilization. pp. 134
Reference written by Kathleen Stewart, Biology 378 (Mammalogy), University of Wisconsin – Stevens Point. Edited by Chris Yahnke. Page last updated August 8, 2005.